Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
2018, Dialogs of DynamEq Inner & Outer Peace-building
Foundational practices are mutual branch-science investigations for a broad bandwidth of modes in Contemplative Science teacher training. Branch disciplines are interlinked by a consensual ethics, worldview, and way of life, as fundamentally correlated to empirical statements and principles. Earlier Publication was via SciCVN.org website and versions as evidences on B. Alan Wallace's Wikipedia Talk page, circa 2015 The category of Inner and Outer Peace-building and West/East historical and contemporary Inner and Outer Science, further qualifies a genre in the documentation, primarily of foundational dimensions of praxis to established empirical discovery during half a century of present-centred results as ongoing phases of experimentation.
Karl Jaspers Forum, Target Article 2, 17 July 1997. Online journal
[1] ABSTRACT: The Science of Consciousness (SOC) is continuous with everyday thinking and with other scientific specialities in beginning inevitably with the inquiring subject's own conscious experiencing. This does not lead to solipsism, because the hypothesis of an independently existing world is the best hypothesis to explain the facts of subjective experience. SOC is unique among all ways of knowing in needing to be fully critical, not simply as academic philosophy is by conceptualizing the structure of conscious inquiry, but by being reflectively aware of consciousness as such, the womb from which inquiry is born. Therefore, in SOC the scientist and the philosopher merge. Initially, this reflective awareness means being open to experiencing non-naturalistic as well as naturalistic claims, altered states of consciousness as well as ordinary ones. It is an empirical issue, not to be decided a priori by some empiricist commitment, whether such non-naturalistic claims and altered states actually exist and what their relevance is to understanding consciousness
British Journal of Psychology, 1999
The growth of research on consciousness creates an opportunity to enrich psychological practice. This paper suggests that a more even balance between thirdand ®rst-person perspectives may now be sought, and that this will make possible more informed interaction between psychological science and pre-scienti®c traditions for investigating the mind. This paper concludes by discussing how such developments might enhance the image of psychological science.
Humankind faces a variety of pressing and significant challenges, and an array of tremendous opportunities. Surprisingly, upgrading our current approach to consciousness science will be a crucial part of navigating this 21st-century social, economic, and ecological territory. [Chapter 1 from 'The Science We Need - One Experiment to Change the World'.]
2018
At the birth of psychology as a science, consciousness was its central problem. But throughout the twentieth century, ideological and methodological concerns pushed the explicit empirical study of consciousness to the sidelines. Since the 1990s, studying consciousness has regained a legitimacy and impetus befitting its status as the central feature of our mental lives. Nowadays consciousness science encompasses a rich interdisciplinary mixture drawing together philosophical, theoretical, computational, experimental, and clinical perspectives. While solving the metaphysically ‘hard’ problem of why consciousness is part of the universe may seem as intractable as ever, scientists have learned a great deal about the neural mechanisms underlying conscious states. Further progress will depend on specifying closer explanatory mappings between (first person subjective) phenomenological descriptions and (third person objective) descriptions of biological and physical processes. Such progress...
1975
Additionally, I am beholden to over a hundred other individuals who have freely revealed to me many of their deepest thoughts in the course of the Thinking Allowed and InnerWork video interviews which serve as a general background to the revised edition. We have myths and stories. We have world views, paradigms, constructs and hypotheses. We have competing dogmas, theologies and sciences. Do we have understanding? Can an integration of our scientific knowledge with the spiritual insights of humanity bring greater harmony to human civilization? We go about our business. We build cities and industries. We engage in buying and selling. We have families and raise children. We affiliate with religious teachings or other traditions. We sometimes avoid confronting the deep issues of being because there we feel insecure, even helpless. And like a mirror of our inner being, our society reflects our tension. Yet the mystery of being continues to rear its head. It will not go away. As we face ecological disaster, nuclear war, widespread drug addiction, widespread inhumanity, we are forced to notice the consequences of our lives in ever greater detail. Are not these horrendous situations the products of human consciousness and behavior? Can we any longer continue to address the major political, technological and social issues of our time without also examining the roots of our consciousness and our behavior? Can we reconcile our spiritual and material natures? Can we discover a cultural unity underlying the diverse dogmas, religions, and political systems on our planet? This book suggests we have that potential. It details the the progress of some who have dared to probe the roots of being. Let us now begin the journey of discovery together. References. Paradoxically, one might also say that the reverse is true: the only thing that we know objectively and directly is our own consciousness. The rest is all secondary. The great physicist, Sir Arthur Eddington, put it this way: Primarily the sphere of objective law is the interplay of thoughts, emotions, memories and volitions in consciousness. The resolution of this paradox, that consciousness might be both objective and subjective, will be the focus of a detailed discussion in the Appendix.
Michael Winkelman, Arizona State University (retired), [email protected] word count (main text) = 4495 Abstract Consciousness involves the integrative functioning of awareness, sensations, perceptions, memory, cognitions, sense of self and worldviews. Anthropology studies cross-cultural differences in these basic elements of consciousness to illustrate the interaction of biological and cultural factors in the development of human consciousness. These interactions produce cross-cultural variation in consciousness in the imprinting of cultural influences into biological development and brain structures. Cognitive approaches place culture at the focus of consciousness research by showing how the conceptual categorization provided by cultural schemas play determinant roles in experiences. Anthropology expands understanding of the variation in manifestations of consciousness in shamanistic phenomena such as soul flight, mystical states and possession. The anthropology of consciousness includes the evolution of consciousness, the role of language in consciousness, the manifestations of class-based, social, ethnic and other forms of consciousness, and the application of ethnographic findings and models of consciousness to archeological interpretation and modern concerns of healing.
1992
The following article reports on ideas about how to study consciousness that emerged during the course of the January 1992 Athens Symposium on Science and Consciousness, one of the principal aims of the meeting being as far as possible to escape from constraints on thinking about consciousness that might be imposed by conventional modes of thought. The first half of the report discusses in general terms the question of opening up the mind to wider ways of thinking, and this is followed by a more detailed compendium of concepts and specific ways of proceeding.
Journal of Psychology & Clinical Psychiatry
Consciousness has proven difficult to define. One approach is to recognize it requires several different component 'prongs'. Neppe and Close had published on the EPIC prongs, now standing for Essence, Paradigmatic Levels, Information-Meaning, and Cybernetic Consciousness. While these were demonstrably valuable in one Neuropsychiatry study, the prongs require further amplifications and hence eight other have been introduced.
A fully-functioning consciousness science is vital for humankind's navigation of the 21st century. Unfortunately the field currently has a number of significant dysfunctions. Fortunately, they're all eminently fixable! However, there's very little attention currently either to the deep roots of problems, or to fixes. Notably, there's a crucial experiment that needs to be done, if we're to have any kind of scientific approach to conscious experience ... This Chapter ends with an explicit strategy for engaging with, and transforming, the current field. [Chapter 3 from 'The Science We Need - One Experiment to Change the World'.]
2007
This excellent book is aptly titled. It presents a closely argued analysis of the current state of consciousness studies and suggests a strategy of investigation, which the author believes is necessary to establish a robust science of consciousness. Before he introduces the details of his framework for a ...
The downfall of structuralist schools of psychology in the early-twenieth century is a well-known piece of cognitive science folklore. The ‘introspectionists’, as their detractors called them, intended to use scientific methods to map the world of sensory experience, but their research programme collapsed somewhat abruptly when it was discovered that the results of the various structuralist labs were incommensurable. If the stronger conclusions of Elizabeth Irvine’s first book, Consciousness as Scientific Concept, are correct, we might one day regard contemporary scientific research into consciousness as we presently regard introspectionism: a troubled chapter in the history of cognitive science. Irvine’s thesis is that the concept of consciousness should be eliminated from scientific practice....
Journal of Social Sciences, 2021
Contemporary science, in recent decades, reflects intensely on the phenomenon of consciousness. This fact is due to the accelerated development of cognitive sciences, biological and physical sciences, neuroscience, which have achieved certain successes in researching the problem of mind-body, consciousness. However, what is strictly required is the issue of the possibility of a scientific theory of consciousness, which would apply a new research methodology. The most recent approaches in this direction substantiate the need for research from a phenomenological structural perspective, which explains consciousness as a phenomenon determined by the subquantum level. Structural-phenomenological theory holds that this level is a profound reality regulated by specific principles and laws that make consciousness possible.
Dialogs: DynamEq, of InnerOuter Peace-building
Inclusions to this document saliently include primary branch-science background-traditions of the Prana and Qi systems' DynamEq interface (5 decades of research-experimentation), of foundational-background medically-qualified research for Consciousness Studies. This also highlights the lexical Indo-European precedent connotations as apt foundational communal-Intercultural Hermeneutic-studies. Background foundational Arts traditions of this research are of fathomed elucidation,. as authentically originating from intercultural accolades of the Theatre-and-Dance-Movement tradition. This is documented with rejoinders to source on specific curricula outlining the primary projects and protocoled dialog of Consciousness Studies.
Anthropology of consciousness, 1994
In recent years there has been an explosion of scientific work on consciousness in cognitive neuroscience, psychology, and other fields. It has become possible to think that we are moving toward a genuine scientific understanding of conscious experience. But what is the science of consciousness all about, and what form should such a science take? This chapter gives an overview of the agenda. 1 First-person Data and Third-person Data The task of a science of consciousness, as I see it, is to systematically integrate two key classes of data into a scientific framework: third-person data, or data about behavior and brain processes, and first-person data, or data about subjective experience. When a conscious system is observed from the third-person point of view, a range of specific behavioral and neural phenomena present themselves. When a conscious system is observed from the firstperson point of view, a range of specific subjective phenomena present themselves. Both sorts of phenomena have the status of data for a science of consciousness. Third-person data concern the behavior and the brain processes of conscious systems. These behavioral and neurophysiological data provide the traditional material of interest for cognitive psychology and of cognitive neuroscience. Where the science of consciousness is concerned, some particularly relevant third-person data are those having to do with the following: • Perceptual discrimination of external stimuli • The integration of information across sensory modalities • Automatic and voluntary actions Published in (M. Gazzaniga, ed) The Cognitive Neurosciences III. MIT Press, 2004.
Experimental and Theoretical Studies of Consciousness: Ciba Foundation Symposium 174. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, 1993
""Classical ways of viewing the relation of consciousness to the brain and physical world make it difficult to see how consciousness can be a subject of scientific study. In contrast to physical events, it seems to be private, subjective, and viewable only from a subject's first-person perspective. But much of psychology does investigate human experience, which suggests that classical ways of viewing these relations must be wrong. An alternative, Reflexive model is outlined along with it's consequences for methodology. Within this model the external phenomenal world is viewed as part-of consciousness, rather than apart-from it. Observed events are only "public" in the sense of "private experience shared." Scientific observations are only "objective" in the sense of "intersubjective." Observed phenomena are only "repeatable" in the sense that they are sufficiently similar to be taken for "tokens" of the same event "type." This closes the gap between physical and psychological phenomena. Indeed, events out-there in the world can often be regarded as either physical or psychological depending on the network of relationships under consideration. However, studying the experience of other human beings raises further complications. A subject (S) and an experimenter (E) may have symmetrical access to events out-there in the world, but their access to events within the subject's body or brain is asymmetrical (E's third-person perspective vs. S's first-person perspective). Insofar as E and S each have partial access to such events their perspectives are complementary. Access to S's experience is also asymmetrical, but in this case S has exclusive access whereas E can only infer its existence. This has not prevented the systematic investigation of experience, including quantification within psychophysics, psychometrics, and so on. Systematic investigation merely requires that experiences be potentially shared, intersubjective and repeatable. In this the conditions for a science of consciousness are no different to a science of physics." Note added for 2012 Academia.edu upload: This paper, presented at a Ciba Foundation Symposiumin 2002 was the first time the epistemological implications of reflexive monism for a science of consciousness were presented to a group of internationally recognised scholars on consciousness, including John Searle, Dan Dennett, Thomas Nagel, Sydney Shoemaker, Colin McGinn, Michael Lockwood, Margaret Boden, Bernie Baars, Peter Fenwick, Michael Gazzaniga, Jeffrey Gray, Stevan Harnad, Marcel Kinsbourne, Nick Humphrey, John Kihlstrom, Ben Libet, Tony Marcel, Jerome Singer, Robert Van Gulick, Howard Shevrin, and Pat Wall. The discussion that follows the paper is of particular historical interest, much of it focusing on the how to interpret the projected nature (or out-thereness) of much of the phenomenal world. Over the following decade, various participants accepted the importance of the out-thereness of the phenomenal world (e.g. Libet, Gray, and Humphrey) along with other theorists such as Lehar, Revonsuo, Hoche, and Tye. However, whether phenomena that seem to be out in the world are really in the brain continued to be a source of contention.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.