Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
This paper proposes to discuss the basic aspects of language formation in the brain – its location, before proceeding to discuss aphasia. The paper draws upon the research carried out at University of Liverpool, where it was shown that reading Shakespeare increases intelligence. This research takes current findings further in the field of Neurolinguistics, by showing how the effects of reading Shakespeare can affect people suffering from various forms of aphasia. The research is based on the findings of the author in classroom context, and the subjects of the research are his students suffering from some form of language disorder. The paper does not only list the findings, but attempts an explanation of why and how reading Shakespeare can be medically beneficial, by revealing some of the basic features of neuroanatomy, certain chemicals which are at work at certain places, and they can affect language, memory and speech production.
Published online xxx
History of Psychiatry, 2006
This paper presents a comparative analysis of the research on aphasia carried out by the linguist Roman Jakobson and the neuropsychiatrist Kurt Goldstein. The linguistic theory of aphasia advocated by Jakobson in the 1950s and 1960s is based on clinical case studies reported by Goldstein at the beginning of the 1930s. However, Jakobson used Goldstein’s clinical observations without taking into account his theoretical work on language pathology. In particular, Jakobson fed the symptoms described by Goldstein into a structuralist model, allowing him to predict different types of aphasia deductively. Goldstein, however, saw the clinical manifestations of aphasia as a particular way of being in the world. By studying the changes associated with the patient’s reaction to the disease, Goldstein wanted to reach an understanding of language functioning in the normal subject. He distinguished between an instrumental use and a symbolic use of language, the latter mainly characteristic of lang...
Brain and Cognition, 2004
Two aphasic patients speaking Brazilian Portuguese and presenting the characteristics of Broca's aphasics were analyzed in relation to their capacities to express null subjects in two different grammatical persons: 1st and 3rd persons. The analysis consisted of looking at the means obtained from the two aphasic patients and their individual results concerning the production of null subjects in each grammatical person on focus. The individual pattern shown by each patient indicated the relevancy of analyzing individually the performance of neuropsychological patients. In this way the hypothesis which claims that in Brazilian Portuguese the null subject has two different natures could be verified.
Aphasiology, 2018
Background: Since Trousseau (1864) introduced the term aphasia to describe the communication impairments that follow brain damage, aphasia has been seen as a disorder of language. Before 1864, most writers on the topic described what we now call aphasia as resulting from a disorder of memory or intelligence. Since the linguistics revolution of the 1960/70s the components of a standard linguistic model of language have been utilized to describe aphasic deficits in terms of impairments in lexical-semantics, syntax, morphology, or phonology. These descriptions have been seen as sufficient explanation for aphasic impairmentsaphasia represents damage to linguistic rules or representations. Research over the past couple of decades has seen a different view on the nature of aphasia emerging. Aim: This presentation reviews contemporary research suggesting that underlying apparent "linguistic" deficits (the symptoms of aphasia) are impairments of more general non-linguistic cognitive mechanisms and processes. I will discuss the proposition that aphasia affects language-specific cognitive operations, but is itself not primarily a disorder of language. Discussion: Such cognitive operations as short-term/working memory, executive functions (e.g., attention, (dis)inhibition, anosognosia), action processing (e.g., apraxia of speech), and emotion (e.g., stress and anxiety) can account for many, maybe most, of the features of aphasia. There is a radical view that proposes that aphasia is a disorder of performance where representations are not "lost". The "loss" of a component of languagephonemes, morphemes, lexical items, syntactic unitsis seen as simplistic (McNeil, 1982; McNeil, Hula, & Sung, 2011). Language is not possible without these supportive cognitive functions and it is these functions that are impaired or compromised, not the rules and representations that are managed by these supportive cognitive operations. A growing number of aphasiologists support the idea that the weight of evidence for a processing account of aphasia is overwhelming and cannot be overlooked; the focus is on the processes and mechanisms responsible for the construction of representations, not the building blocks themselves.
NeuroImage, 2008
2014
The Routledge Handbook of Applied Linguistics, 2024
Neurolinguistics is the study of how first (L1) and additional languages are represented and processed in the brain. It is an interdisciplinary field with significant input from neuroscience, linguistics, psychology, speech-language pathology, and biology. Neurolinguistics plays an important role in the field of second language (L2) acquisition, for instance, as our knowledge of language and brain can have critical implications for how languages are taught and, consequently, learned. In this chapter, we focus on how neurolinguistics informs applied linguistics and in particular, L2 teaching and learning. We will begin by foreshadowing the age-old interest in the brain and whether it (or the heart, for instance) was responsible for language. We then look at how our understanding of brain and language has developed over time, has reached a turning point in the 19th century, and has since sharpened as advanced technologies have been introduced. We then turn to discuss current research topics and their contributions to neuroscientific approaches to applied linguistics, along with the many methods that can be used to study language and brain. The chapter concludes by looking at future directions in this research area and by putting forth practical considerations for teaching L2s in formal settings
JLTonline Reviews, 2011
What would happen if a well-known psycholinguist were to meet a novelist-translator and they decided to work together? They would most likely discuss their common interest and field of work which is language. They would express their point of view on this subject and ...
Pro Sciences: Revista de Producción, Ciencias e Investigación, 2018
This study seeks to give an approximation to the role of the use of neuroscience in the teaching of the English language, to create educational resources to a language with a communicative approach teaching and reflecting on the implementation of methodological strategies congruent to modern education, is made a revision to new contributions in the pedagogical field, which include neural studies to see more closely how it develops and mature central nervous system and the brain, so now that the emphasis on learning a foreign language has become popular in recent decades, arduous way seeks to methodological strategies and activities to enhance their learning, which in this study proposes the use of six teaching strategies based on knowledge of how the brain, which are a contribution to reduce the educational gap between the old and the new school.I conducted this study is with teachers of English from the University of Agraria del Ecuador. The correlational descriptive method is used...
Journal of the Northern Renaissance 5, 2013
Gaceta de M�xico, 2018
(1564-1616) lived during the golden age of English drama (end of Elizabeth I and beginning of James I reign) and is undoubtedly one of the most prolific literary artists of all times. 1,2 In addition to his literary quality, his works express medical knowledge: they contain descriptions that are relevant to medical practice of the time and even to current medicine. 1-4 A descriptive analysis of some characters allows to distinguish disorders of neurological or psychiatric nature in them; many Shakespearean heroes and antiheroes are examples of dementia, parasomnia, parkinsonism, epilepsy, migraine, palsy and even infectious diseases. 5-9 At 400 years of the Bard of Avon's death, his works continue to be a source of inspiration. The amount of clinical data in Shakespeare's works is impressive; he wrote between 35 and 38 theatrical plays, 6 lyrical works and 154 sonnets (approximately), where finding little more than 700 references to medicine of the time is possible. 1,10-12 For this reason, several researchers have analyzed the medical concepts in Shakespearean literature; some agree that they may be the result of his studies, while others indicate that Shakespeare reproduced the medical knowledge of some "men of wisdom" of his time. 9,13 For example, according to Gómes, 1 the Diseases desperate grown, By desperate alliances are relieved, Or not at all.
Journal of Literary Theory, 2009
I would like to summarize the debate so far: Kilian Koepsell and Carlos Spoerhase discuss the problem of knowledge transfer from one discipline to another and the legitimacy of knowledge transfer into literary studies in their position paper ›Cognitive Science and the Study of Literature. Some Thoughts on the Possibility of Transferring Knowledge‹. They admit that, in principle, cognitive processes play a central role, for example, in the reading process. However, the knowledge borrowed from the cognitive sciences should also be of use to literary theory, which means it should »yield results that would not have been readily obtainable there without the transferred knowledge. In this case, the legitimacy of a knowledge transfer would depend on whether it leads to innovation in the receiving discipline« (Koepsell/Spoerhase, 372). They refer to Gerhard Lauer's essay ›Spiegelneuronen: Über den Grund des Wohlgefallens an der Nachahmung‹ [Mirror Neurons: Why We Like to Imitate] (2007). The essay attempts to show how the theory of mirror neuron systems can be fruitful for literary studies. Koepsell and Spoerhase's first objection against Lauer's essay refers to the explicative achievements of researching mirror neurons, their second objection to the explicative scope of Lauer's suggestion to ground literary studies in the human sciences. Because mirror neurons fire when we perform an action, as well as when we see or hear an action, they are the foundation of all our comprehension and imitation processes. According to Lauer, literature also takes part in these processes because it is »food for our imitation instinct« (»Nahrung für unseren Nachahmungsinstinkt«) (Lauer 2007, 152). Kilian Koepsell and Carlos Spoerhase correctly observe that it is not clear yet why the neurons fire. We cannot draw any conclusions about their causes or their mechanisms. Studies on mirror neurons could therefore not explain how empathy is conveyed in literary texts; at the most, they are consistent with theories of empathy or imitation, respectively.
Until now, researchers in the field of neuropsychology have extensively studied the various aspects of brain function and its mechanisms. They have conducted extensive research on brain structure, brain damage, and the loss of brain tissue function associated with diseases. These studies have focused on areas such as language and speech comprehension, attention span, and memory. By examining the language system itself and utilizing animal models, researchers have attempted to determine the role and involvement of language in mental processes, which are closely tied to the brain and nervous system. However, studying the language system in detail from a psycholinguistic and neurobiological perspective is challenging due to the complex anatomical structure of the brain. Nevertheless, the study of aphasia, a language disorder resulting from brain damage or disease that causes an inability to speak, has brought us closer to unraveling the puzzle of language-brain connections and achieving more realistic results. Over a century of research related to the brain and language, including the history of aphasia, has deepened our understanding beyond the classical model. Modern perspectives on language and cognition now explain the concept of brain function and cellular mechanisms in relation to language. It is believed that further research on the pathology of aphasia will provide a clearer understanding of the organization and role of language in the brain.
Journal of US-China Public Administration
Neurolinguistics is known as a study of relationship between brain and language. Human brain consists of three main parts such as forebrain (crucial part), midbrain (the smallest part), and hindbrain (the lowest part). Basically, human language centre is located in frontal lobe of human brain where the left and the right hemispheres are located. Language use sometimes is more dominant in the left hemisphere although the right hemisphere functions for language. Specifically, the areas that are involved in language are frontal lobe (Broca) and temporal lobe (Wernicke). The lesion on brain will scientifically cause language disorder which is known as aphasia. The types of aphasia are Broca's aphasia, Wernicke's aphasia, pure word deafness aphasia, conduction aphasia, anomic aphasia, apraxia aphasia, global aphasia, and aphasia reading and writing (dyslexia). The physical condition of a healthy brain is very important to prepare the activities of thinking and acting. Thus, the brain must be protected appropriately.
Literature suggests that illiterate subjects are unaware of the phonological structure of language. This fact may influence the characteristics of aphasic speech, namely the structure of paraphasias. A battery of tests was developed for this study to be used with aphasic subjects (literate and illiterate), in order to explore this topic in more detail. This article aims to present the experimental design and the results of this test battery composed of two sub-tests: (i) a naming test with words that belong to three distinct groups: high frequency simple words (HFSW), low frequency simple words (LFSW), and low frequency complex words (LFCW); and (ii) a word repetition test. The variables of literacy, frequency and word morphology, and their effect on the performance of aphasic groups, were correlated in this study. Morphology was the variable that exercised the greatest influence on the verbal production of the participants.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.