Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
…
4 pages
1 file
Humans show a remarkable tendency to describe and think of numbers as being placed on a mental number line (MNL), with smaller numbers located on the left and larger ones on the right. Faster responses to small numbers are indeed performed on the left side of space, while responses to large numbers are facilitated on the right side of space (spatial-numerical association of response codes, SNARC effect). This phenomenon is considered the experimental demonstration of the MNL and has been extensively replicated throughout a variety of paradigms. Nevertheless, the majority of previous literature has mainly investigated this effect by means of response times and accuracy, whereas studies considering more subtle and automatic measures such as kinematic parameters are rare (e.g., in a reaching-to-grasp movement, the grip aperture is enlarged in responding to larger numbers than in responding to small numbers). In this brief review we suggest that numerical magnitude can also affect the what and how of action execution (i.e., temporal and spatial components of movement). This evidence could have large implications in the strongly debated issue concerning the effect of experience and culture on the orientation of MNL. In the 19th century Galton first noted that humans visualize and think of numbers as represented on a mental number line (MNL), usually oriented from left-to-right. Along the MNL smaller numbers are located on the left side and larger ones on the right side (Galton, 1880; Dehaene, 2011). The first, and often replicated, experimental demonstration of the MNL is the fact that adult humans are faster at processing small numbers when responses are executed on the left side of space, and at processing large numbers when responses are executed on the right side of space (spatial-numerical association of response codes, SNARC effect; Dehaene et al., 1993). Since this first evidence, large body of studies have investigated the cognitive representation of numbers using chronometric methods, but the impact of number processing on motor control has been scarcely explored. According to a recent influential hypothesis, cognitive representations of perceptual and semantic information cannot be fully understood without considering their impact on actions (Gallese and Lakoff, 2005). In this vein, here we present a critical review to highlight how the existing knowledge on MNL could be fostered by studies that critically analyze the link between motor actions and numbers.
Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 2011
Active head turns to the left and right have recently been shown to influence numerical cognition by shifting attention along the mental number line. In the present study, we found that passive whole-body motion influences numerical cognition. In a random-number generation task (Experiment 1), leftward and downward displacement of participants facilitated small number generation, whereas rightward and upward displacement facilitated the generation of large numbers. Influences of leftward and rightward motion were also found for the processing of auditorily presented numbers in a magnitude-judgment task (Experiment 2). Additionally, we investigated the reverse effect of the number-space association (Experiment 3). Participants were displaced leftward or rightward and asked to detect motion direction as fast as possible while small or large numbers were auditorily presented. When motion detection was difficult, leftward motion was detected faster when hearing small number and rightward motion when hearing large number. We provide new evidence that bottom-up vestibular activation is sufficient to interact with the higher-order spatial representation underlying numerical cognition. The results show that action planning or motor activity is not necessary to influence spatial attention. Moreover, our results suggest that self-motion perception and numerical cognition can mutually influence each other.
Progress in Brain Research, 2016
The relationship between number and space representation is still one of the most debated topics in studies of mathematical cognition. Here we offer a concise review of two important behavioral effects that have pointed out the use of a spatially left-to-right oriented mental number line (MNL) in healthy participants: the SNARC effect and the attentional SNARC effect (Att-SNARC). Following a brief summary of seminal investigations on the introspective properties of the MNL, we review recent empirical evidence and theories on the functional origin of the SNARC effect, where upon left/right response choices faster reaction times are found for small numbers with left-side responses and for large numbers with right-side responses. Then we offer a summary of the studies that have investigated whether the mere perception of visual Arabic numbers presented at central fixation can engender spatially congruent lateral shifts of attention, ie, leftward for small numbers and rightward for large ones, ie, the Att-SNARC effect. Finally, we summarize four experiments that tested whether the Att-SNARC depends on an active rather than passive processing of centrally presented digit cues. In line with other recent studies, these experiment do not replicate the original Att-SNARC and show that the 1 These authors contributed equally to the study.
2016
Perception of number and space are tightly intertwined. It has been proposed that this is due to 'cortical recycling', where numerosity processing takes over circuits originally processing space. Do such 'recycled' circuits retain their original functionality? Here, we investigate interactions between numerosity and motion direction, two functions that both localize to parietal cortex. We describe a new phenomenon in which visual motion direction adapts nonsymbolic numerosity perception, giving rise to a repulsive aftereffect: motion to the left adapts small numbers, leading to overestimation of numerosity, while motion to the right adapts large numbers, resulting in underestimation. The reference frame of this effect is spatiotopic. Together with the tuning properties of the effect this suggests that motion direction-numerosity cross-adaptation may occur in a homolog of area LIP. 'Cortical recycling' thus expands but does not obliterate the functions originally performed by the recycled circuit, allowing for shared computations across domains.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2008
Frontiers in Psychology
In the past decade hand kinematics has been reliably adopted for investigating cognitive processes and disentangling debated topics. One of the most controversial issues in numerical cognition literature regards the origin-cultural vs. genetically driven-of the mental number line (MNL), oriented from left (small numbers) to right (large numbers). To date, the majority of studies have investigated this effect by means of response times, whereas studies considering more culturally unbiased measures such as kinematic parameters are rare. Here, we present a new paradigm that combines a "free response" task with the kinematic analysis of movement. Participants were seated in front of two little soccer goals placed on a table, one on the left and one on the right side. They were presented with left-or right-directed arrows and they were instructed to kick a small ball with their right index toward the goal indicated by the arrow. In a few test trials participants were presented also with a small (2) or a large (8) number, and they were allowed to choose the kicking direction. Participants performed more left responses with the small number and more right responses with the large number. The whole kicking movement was segmented in two temporal phases in order to make a hand kinematics' fine-grained analysis. The Kick Preparation and Kick Finalization phases were selected on the basis of peak trajectory deviation from the virtual midline between the two goals. Results show an effect of both small and large numbers on action execution timing. Participants were faster to finalize the action when responding to small numbers toward the left and to large number toward the right. Here, we provide the first experimental demonstration which highlights how numerical processing affects action execution in a new and not-overlearned context. The employment of this innovative and unbiased paradigm will permit to disentangle the role of nature and culture in shaping the direction of MNL and the role of finger in the acquisition of numerical skills. Last but not least, similar paradigms will allow to determine how cognition can influence action execution.
2008
The tendency to respond faster with the left hand to relatively small numbers and faster with the right hand to relatively large numbers (SNARC effect) has been interpreted as an automatic association of spatial and numerical information. We investigated in two experiments the impact of task-irrelevant memory representations on this effect. Participants memorized three Arabic digits describing a left-to-right ascending number sequence (e.g., 3-4-5), a descending sequence (e.g., 5-4-3) or a disordered sequence (e.g., 5-3-4) and indicated afterwards the parity status of a centrally presented digit (i.e., 1, 2, 8, or 9) with a left/right keypress response. As indicated by the reaction times, the SNARC effect in the parity task was mediated by the coding requirements of the memory tasks. That is, a SNARC effect was only present after memorizing ascending or disordered number sequences but disappeared after processing descending sequences. Interestingly, the effects of the second task were only present if all sequences within one experimental block had the same type of order. Taken together, our findings are inconsistent with the idea that spatial-numerical associations are the result of an automatic and obligatory cognitive process but do suggest that coding strategies might be responsible for the cognitive link between numbers and space. Research in the field of mathematical cognition has accumulated evidence indicating that cognitive representations of numerical magnitudes are closely linked with representations of space. A striking demonstration of this connection is the so called effect of the spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC effect), which reflects the tendency of participants to respond faster with the left hand toward relatively small numbers and to respond faster with the right hand toward relatively large numbers . This interaction between number size and spatial response features has been consistently interpreted as evidence that numerical magnitude information is spatially coded and associated with a mental continuum ("mental number line") on which numbers are consecutively arranged in an ascending order from the left side to the right (for recent reviews see, e.g., Hubbard,
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2014
A dominant hypothesis on how the brain processes numerical size proposes a spatial representation of numbers as positions on a 'mental number line'. An alternative hypothesis considers numbers as elements of a generalized representation of sensorimotor-related magnitude which is not obligatorily spatial. Here we show that individuals' relative use of spatial and non-spatial representations has a cerebral counterpart in the structural organization of the posterior parietal cortex. Inter-individual variability in the linkage between numbers and spatial responses (faster left responses to low numbers and right responses to high numbers; SNARC effect) correlated with variations in grey matter volume around the right precuneus. Conversely, differences in the disposition to link numbers to force production (faster soft responses to low numbers and hard responses to high numbers) were related to grey matter volume in the left angular gyrus. This finding suggests that numerical cognition relies on multiple mental representations of analogue magnitude using different neural implementations that are linked to individual traits.
2010
A tight correspondence has been postulated between the representations of number and space. The spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect, which reflects the observation that people respond faster with the left-hand side to small numbers and with the right-hand side to large numbers, is regarded as strong evidence for this correspondence. The dominant explanation of the SNARC effect is that it results from visuospatial coding of magnitude (e.g., the mental number line hypothesis). In a series of experiments, we demonstrated that this is only part of the story and that verbal-spatial coding influences processes and representations that have been believed to be purely visuospatial. Additionally, when both accounts were directly contrasted, verbal-spatial coding was observed in absence of visuospatial coding. Relations to other number-space interactions and implications for other tasks are discussed.
Current Biology, 2004
The parietal cortex is a central part of the brain's system for representing numbers and magnitudes. Activity in the parietal cortex might reflect number representation or actions made in response to the numbers.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2008
eLife, 2016
Perception & Psychophysics, 2009
Journal of Vision, 2014
Neuropsychologia, 2006
Memory & Cognition, 2007
Frontiers in Psychology, 2012
PsycEXTRA Dataset
Experimental Psychology (formerly Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie), 2013
Frontiers in Psychology
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2014
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2015