Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
This thesis is concerned with the following basic question. Given the fact that causatives across languages (and sometimes within the same language) may have morphological or syntactic realisation, how do we account for the semantic as well as syntactic behaviour of the morphological causative? Specifically, is it tenable to adopt the view that one is derived from the other on the basis that they show some degree of synonymy? How do we then explain the different properties of the two causatives, particularly the fact that the morphological causative patterns with non-derived words? Is there any explanation for the non-compositionality and the associated different GF-mapping processes exhibited by some derived causatives? The answers for these questions emerge from a detailed analysis of the Indonesian -kan causative. The causative phenomena are consistently approached from two opposing angles: syntactic (the GB-based Incorporation Theory) and lexical (LFG). I argue that the various properties of the -kan causative can be explained in terms of the lexical approach. The morphological causative is better not viewed as being derived from the periphrastic one. The morphological causative is formed within the morphological component of the lexicon. Following Mohanan (1990, 1993), the information carried by the derived word emerging from the lexicon is assumed to be distributed over four levels of structure: the Logical Conceptual Structure (LC STR), the Argument Structure (ARG STR), the Grammatical Category Structure (GC STR) and the Grammatical Function Structure (GF STR). I propose a mechanism of word formation within the framework of LFG. Its machinery basically consists of the word-schema (W-structure), the Categorial and Functional Signature (CFS) and its associated Percolation Conventions, and the Rule of Inference (RI). The W- structure rule is the rule accounting for the structure below Xo which respect the X- bar schema. The CFS and the Percolation Conventions regulate the flow of features and PRED values passing up to the mother node constituting the features and PRED value(s) of the derived word. The RI accounts for the predictable occurrences of PRED values in the derived forms which are not strictly compositional. Another essential part of the lexicon is the LMT principles regulating the GF-mapping. The study supports the Lexical Integrity Hypothesis and reveals that the interaction among the principles of the word-formation mechanism can neatly explain the semantic as well the grammatical properties of the morphological causative -kan.
Lingua, 2010
In this paper, I explore the issue of the division of labor between syntax and morphology within the context of the lexicalist vs. nonlexicalist debate on the basis of case studies of phrasal compounds in English and ber-constructions in Indonesian. I first show what challenges these phenomena raise for various existing versions of the lexicalist theory to have a clear grasp of what aspect(s) of the theory must be dropped or improved upon. I then propose (non-lexicalist) alternative accounts of the two phenomena. I show that phrasal compounds can be explained on a par with regular compounding of two simplex roots as a natural consequence of the Multiple Spell-Out model of the Minimalist Program. I provide evidence that ber-constructions in Indonesian are derived via head movement, rejecting potential alternative lexicalist accounts in terms of lexical compounding. I also briefly discuss several architectural design specifications that any model of the morphology-syntax interface must meet. I conclude that the firewall theory of the interface, which determines the degree of the interpenetration between syntax and morphology on a language-particular basis, not only meets these specifications, but also serves as an explanatory model within which the syntax-morphology interaction can be productively pursued. Crown
Lingua, 2014
Argument structure in morphology and syntax: An introduction 1. Where do arguments come from?
Abstract During the past 40 years research of causativity belonged to the central themes of the general and comparative resp. typological linguistics. In this respect it is astonishing, that in my opinion from the Slavic side this subject was treated if at all very marginally in the past. My interest was motivated by the fact that Causatives and Anti-Causatives require an analysis, which touches an interface of morphology, semantics, lexicon, word formation and syntax. Therefore it is also replicable by the Minimalistic Program (with the inclusion of Distributive Morphology). Furthermore, the theme comprises important observations concerning questions of affix ordering, syntactic structures and verb movement. Most syntactic accounts of affix ordering and verb movement follow the theory of incorporation by Mark Baker (1988). In this theory syntactic incorporation is assumed to be an instance of the general syntactic rule Move Alpha, e.g. a syntactic operation that derives morphologically complex words from morphologically basic elements (root, stems, or affixes) by head to head movement via incorporation. Whereas the traditional view on morphology and word formation is that word-formation takes place in lexicon, and that morphological rules are different in nature and apply on different primitive elements than syntactic rules, we shall try to advocate an analysis in which the phenomenon of Anti-Causatives and Causatives has to be derived from different ROOT-Semantics of verbs projecting different trees and syntactic structures by the operations AGREE and MERGE. The Causative Alternation (CAL) will serve as criteria to distinguish between externally and internally caused causation; with help of the CAL the Unaccusativity will be divided into two subgroups: alternating Unaccusative (AU-) verbs and non-alternating Unaccusative (NAU-) verbs. In the following an alternate distinction between AU- and NAU-verbs will be developed, namely the presence/absence of information about how the process to be treated was caused. The universal concept of the encyclopedic lexicon in English, German, Russian and Czech (partly also other European and non European languages) seems to assume four different ROOTS of verbs at base to classify the Anti-Causativity-Opposition: √ agentive (murder, assassinate,cut), √ internally caused (blossom, wilt, grow), √ externally caused (destroy, kill, slay) √cause unspecified (break, open,melt). Moreover, it will be shown that unergative/causative pairs depict an independent phenomenon, which does not affect considerations about CAL (correspondent to Alexiadou et al. 2006a, b, Kosta 2010, 2011; and Marantz 1997, but dissenting Levin & Rappaport Hovav 1995 and Reinhart 2000). This talk is organized as follows: in section 1, I propose a formulation of the MP based on syntactic features; the examples will be taken from Causatives and Anti-Causatives that are derived by affixes (in Russian, Czech, Polish, German, English and some other languages of different types and origins) by head-to-head movement. In section 2, I review some basic facts in support of a syntactic approach to Merge of Causatives and Anti-Causatives, proposing that theta roles are also syntactic Features that merge functional affixes with their stems in a well-defined way. I first try to give some external evidence in showing that Causatives and Anti-Causatives obey a principle of thematic hierarchy early postulated in generative literature by Jackendoff (1972; 43), and later reformulated in terms of argument-structure-ordering principle by Grimshaw (1990:chapter 2). Crucial for my paper is the working hypothesis that every syntactic theory which tries to capture the data not only descriptively but also explanatively should descend from three levels of syntactic representation: a-structure where the relation between predicate and its arguments (and adjuncts) takes place, thematic structure where the theta-roles are assigned to their arguments, and event structure, which decides about the aspectual distribution and division of events.
2013
Analytic causatives are the type of causatives formed by separate predicates expressing the cause and the effect, that is, the causing notion is realized by a word separate from the word denoting the caused activity. This paper aims to discuss the forms and syntactic structure of analytic causatives in Javanese. To discuss the syntactic structure, the theory of lexical functional grammar (LFG) is employed. The data used in this study is the "ngoko" level of Javanese of the Surakarta dialect. By using a negation marker and modals as the syntactic operators to test mono-or bi-clausality of analytic causatives, the writer found that analytic causatives in Javanese form biclausal constructions. These constructions have an X-COMP structure, in that the SUBJ of the second verb is controlled by the OBJ of the causative verb (N)gawe "make". In terms of the constituent structure, analytic causatives have two kinds of structures, which are V-cause OBJ X-COMP and V-cause X-...
The Lexical Semantic Framework (LSF, Lieber, 2004) is concerned with the study of the semantics of word-formation processes. The central goal of LSF is to characterize the meaning of simple lexemes and affixes and to show how these meanings can be integrated in the creation of complex words. LSF offers a systematic treatment of issues that figure prominently in the study of word formation: (a) The polysemy question: why do derivational affixes often exhibit polysemy (e.g. agent, instrument, experiencer, stimulus, patient/theme nouns in-er, as in driver, opener, hearer, pleaser, keeper)? (b) The multiple-affix question: why are there affixes that create the same kind of derived words (i.e. bother and-ant create agent nouns, e.g. writer, accountant)? (c) The zero-derivation question: how do we account for zero-affixation, that is, semantic change with no (overt) formal change (i.e. " conversion ")? (d) The form and meaning mismatches question: why are there instances where the form and meaning correlation is often not one-to-one? LSF has its source in a confluence of research approaches that follow a decompositional approach to meaning and, thus, defines simple lexemes and affixes by way of a systematic representation that is achieved via a constrained formal language that enforces consistency of annotation. Lexical-semantic representations in LSF consist of two parts: the Semantic/Grammatical Skeleton and the Semantic/Pragmatic Body (henceforth 'skeleton' and 'body' respectively). The skeleton is comprised of features that are of relevance to the syntax. These features act as functions and may take arguments. Functions and arguments of a skeleton are hierarchically arranged. The body encodes all those aspects of meaning that are perceptual, cultural, and encyclopedic. Features in LSF are used in (a) a cross-categorial, (b) an equipollent, and (c) a priva-tive way. This means that they are used to account for the distinction between the major ontological categories, may have a binary (i.e. positive or negative) value and may or may not form part of the skeleton of a given lexeme. In order to account for the fact that several distinct parts integrate into a single referential unit that projects its arguments to the syntax, LSF makes use of the Principle of Co-indexation. Co-indexation is a device needed in order to tie together the arguments that come with different parts of a complex word to yield only those arguments that are syntactically active. LSF has an important impact on the study of the morphology-lexical semantics interface and provides a unitary theory of meaning in word formation.
Diachronica, 2004
Modern Japanese has a morphological causative, formed by suffixes on the verb, and lexical causatives. The morphological causative has been in use since Late Old Japanese. However, the etymology of this morphological causative and the status of related causative formations in Old Japanese remain unclear. This paper supports the view that lexical causative formations in Old Japanese are the direct predecessors of the morphological causative. In their morphological, syntactic, and semantic features they form a chain of morphologization with the productive causative that emerged from them. Similar diachronic developments have also been observed in Sanskrit and North American languages. Thus, the formation of a morphological causative from a lexical pattern, a path of development that has received little attention, seems to constitute a crosslinguistically valid source for the evolution of productive causatives. It is proposed that the type of change observed here is an exaptation of fossilized morphological material, which, in several important aspects, runs counter to the directionality of change posited in mainstream grammaticalization theory.
Revista española de lingüística aplicada, 2006
Grammar. Such a model deals with word formation processes from two perspectives, as a grammaticalization of the lexicon and as a lexicalization of predication structures. The first view is essentially lexicological in nature, whereas the second one is essentially syntactic. The analyses carried out in this paper will concentrate on the lexicological aspect of the model; specifically, a new proposal for the semantic representation of word formation processes is devised. Such a system of lexical representation involves designing Lexical Templates for both free and bound lexical morphemes and making use of Semantic Redundancy Rules to account for the different semantic values that a derivational pattern may have.
We present a precise LFG-based analysis of the suffix -i in Indonesian, addressing the issues of applicative-causative polysemy of the suffix and its alternation with -kan. We also show how the analysis can be integrated into an implementation of an existing computational grammar. Our computational implementation of applicativisation and related phenomena is the first of its kind for Indonesian, and also provides evidence for the robustness of LFG as a theory and XLE as a computational implementation of the theory in handling this linguistically complex phenomena.
The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Interfaces, 2007
According to the traditional view, the relation between morphology and syntax is the following: while morphology builds up word forms-typically by combining roots with other roots and with affixes, but also by applying other operations to them, syntax takes fully inflected words as input and combines them into phrases and sentences. The division of labour between morphology and syntax is thus perfect: morphology only operates below the word level whereas syntax only operates above the word level.
ENGLISH LINGUISTICS, 2000
2022
Our goal is to develop a semantic theory that is equally suitable for the lexical material (words) and for the larger constructions (sentences) put together from these. In 2.1 we begin with the system of lexical categories that are in generative grammar routinely used as preterminals mediating between syntax and the lexicon. Morphology is discussed in 2.2, where subdirect composition is introduced. This notion is further developed in 2.3, where the geometric view is expanded from the standard word vectors and the voronoids introduced in Chapter 1 to include non-vectorial elements that express binary relations. These eigenspace techniques receive further use in 2.4, where some crucial relational devices of syntactic theory, thematic relations, deep cases, and kārakas are addressed. How much of syntax can be reconstructed with these is discussed in 2.5. 2.1 Lexical categories and subcategories Whether a universal system of lexical categories exists is still a widely debated question. Bloomfield, 1933, and more recently Kaufman, 2009 argued that certain languages like Tagalog have only one category. But the notion that there are at least three major categories that are universal, nouns, verbs, and adjectives, has been broadly defended (Baker, 2003; Chung, 2012; Haspelmath, 2021). 4lang subdivides verbs into two categories: intransitive U and transitive V; retaining the standard N for noun; A for adjective; and also uses D for aDverb; and G for Grammatical formative. While this rough categorization has proven useful for seeking bindings in the original 4 and in other languages, there is no theoretical claim associated to these categories, nei
Parole Journal of Linguistics and Education, 2014
This paper is a study of analytic causatives in Javanese from a Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) perspective. Analytic causatives are the type of causatives where there are separate predicates expressing the cause and the effect, that is, the causing notion is realized by a word separate from the word denoting the caused activity. The question addressed in this paper is whether analytic causatives form a mono-or bi-clausal structure. In addition, this paper aims to explain the mechanism of argument sharing between the verbs involved in analytic causatives. By using a negation marker and modals as the syntactic operators to test mono-or bi-clausality of analytic causatives, it was found that analytic causatives in Javanese are biclausal. These constructions have an X-COMP structure, in that the SUBJ of the second verb is controlled by the OBJ of the causative verb (N)gawe ‗make'. The syntactic structures of analytic causatives are described within a constituent structure and a functional structure, two main components of LFG.
Causativization can only be applied to intransitives in many languages. Some may even restrict it to inagentive intransitives. This paper provides a plausible answer to the question why languages behave like this. It is argued that causative constructions and causativization should be understood primarily in semantic terms, not solely in terms of valency increase and adding a causer. Evidence is provided for a basic common function of causatives and passives (deactorization) that explains the observation that in a number of languages, causatives and passives are marked in the same way.
The paper presents a model of morphosemantic patterns based on the model of Guiraud’s morphosemantic fields. The main reason for introducing this model into the description of the architecture of the Croatian lexicon is the fact that Croatian is a morphologically rich language, in which grammatical and semantic mechanisms interact in lexical organization. The model of morphosemantic patterns at this stage of its development consists of two basic models: the model of morphosemantic fields and the model of morphosemantic grounds. Although the model is based on structuralist tenets, it is our intention to demonstrate how it is related to some of the most prominent contemporary theoretical frameworks, namely Cognitive Linguistics and Construction Grammar, especially Construction Morphology.
Language Literacy: Journal of Linguistics, Literature, and Language Teaching
This article discusses causative construction of Deli Javanese dialect which focuses on morphological and analytic causative construction. This study aims to reveal how morphological and analytic causative constructions in Deli Javanese dialect are formed. The theory of causative construction concept is based on Comrie (1989). The research data were obtained from text books, journals and interview. The causative construction in this analysis was elaborated by using tree diagram. The result showed that the morphological causative construction of Deli Javanese dialect utilized the verbs, which were shown by prefixes and suffixes. The causative construction in the verb nggodoke ‘boil something for somebody’ is derived from nggodok ‘boil’; thus, it is indicated by the suffix –e. Meanwhile, the analytic causative construction utilized nggawe ‘make’ in which it indicates an action with desire. Furthermore, Analytic causative construction in Deli Javanese dialect is a clause construction...
In order to understand the structure, procedure, and causative marking of the Dawan language, this paper addresses causative construction in the Dawan language. Since the writer is a native speaker of the language, the primary data from the field study was corroborated by written materials like the bible in the Dawan language as well as by intuitive data. The elicitation technique combined the two sets of data to obtain valid data. The outcome demonstrates four distinct forms of causative construction: affixation, complex sentences, verb serialisation, and the addition of a causer argument. The transitive form, derived from an intransitive clause, adds the causer argument. S is transferred to the O function, and a causer argument is added to the A function. The conjunction natuin or fun indicates causative marking in a complex sentence. This study considers the Dawan language's causative verb serialization is the most fruitful construction. There are two types of this construction: causative serialisation and causeeffect serialisation. In the first type, V1 is an action verb with the lexical meaning "causative," V2 is the outcome (effect) of what V1 has done. In contrast to the first, the causative verbs moe/mo'e "make" and eik "bring" occupy V1 in causative serialisation, while the outcome of the action in V1 occupies V2. The final method is causative construction by affixation, or the morphological process.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.