Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
2000, Memory & Cognition
…
18 pages
1 file
The temporal relations among candidate causes were studied in a causal induction task using a design that is known to produce occasion setting in animal learning preparations. For some subset of the observations, one event, the occasion setter, was accompanied by another event, the conditional cause; for another subset ofthe observations, the conditional cause occurred alone. The efficacy of the conditional cause depended on whether it was or was not accompanied by the occasion setter. Participants used the occasion setter to modulate their effect expectancy to the conditional cause when the events were presented serially, but not simultaneously. Current causal induction models are unable to account for the full range of effects that we observed; the relative roles of time, attention, and cue distinctiveness are discussed.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2015
When the temporal interval or delay separating cause and effect is consistent over repeated instances, it becomes possible to predict when the effect will follow from the cause, hence temporal predictability serves as an appropriate term for describing consistent cause-effect delays. It has been demonstrated that in instrumental action-outcome learning tasks, enhancing temporal predictability by holding the cause-effect interval constant elicits higher judgements of causality compared to conditions involving variable temporal intervals. Here, we examine whether temporal predictability exerts a similar influence when causal learning takes place through observation rather than intervention through instrumental action. Four experiments demonstrated that judgements of causality were higher when the temporal interval was constant than when it was variable, and that judgements declined with increasing variability. We further found that this beneficial effect of predictability was stronger in situations where the effect base-rate was zero (Experiments 1 and 3). The results therefore clearly indicate that temporal predictability enhances impressions of causality, and that this effect is robust and general. Factors that could mediate this effect are discussed.
… Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section B, 1998
Two experiments on human causal induction with multiple candidate causes are reported. Experiment 1 investigated the in¯uence of a perfect preventive cause on the ratings of a less contingent cause. Whereas the Rescorla± Wagner model (RWM) and Cheng' s probabilistic contrast model predict that the less contingent cause should be completely discounted, the Pearce model predicts, in most cases, an enhancement of that cause' s perceived importance. Results corresponded more closely to the predictions of the Pearce model. T he predictions of both the RWM and the Pearce model rely on a constant context cue acquiring associative strength, yet no such cue was explicitly identi® ed in the task scenario employed in Experiment 1. Experiment 2 replicated a number of key conditions of Experiment 1 with a task scenario that afforded ratings of the causal importance of the context in which the effectiveness of the discrete candidate causes was evaluated. In addition, the number of trials was increased to test the possibility that the ratings in Experiment 1 were the product of incomplete learning. T he results of the ® rst experiment were replicated and the ratings of the effectiveness of the context cue were anticipated by both the RWM and the Pearce model. Overall, the Pearce model offers a more comprehensive account of the causal inferences recorded in this study. In a causal induction task a number of candidate causes may precede the occurrence or absence of a target outcome. T he nature of the inductive inferences supported by one such candidate cause is in part determined by its predictive value as de® ned by the difference between the probability of the outcome given its presence, P(O | C), and given its absence, P(O | 2
How do humans discover causal relations when the effect is not immediately observable? Previous experiments have uniformly demonstrated detrimental effects of outcome delays on causal induction. These findings seem to conflict with everyday causal cognition, where humans can apparently identify long-term causal relations with relative ease. Three experiments investigated whether the influence of delay on adult human causal judgments is mediated by experimentally induced assumptions about the timeframe of the causal relation in question, as suggested by Einhorn & Hogarth (1986). Causal judgments generally decreased when a delay separated cause and effect. This
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2002
In predictive causal inference, people reason from causes to effects, whereas in diagnostic inference, they reason from effects to causes. Independently of the causal structure of the events, the temporal structure of the information provided to a reasoner may vary (e.g., multiple events followed by a single event vs. a single event followed by multiple events). The authors report 5 experiments in which causal structure and temporal information were varied independently. Inferences were influenced by temporal structure but not by causal structure. The results are relevant to the evaluation of 2 current accounts of causal induction, the Rescorla-Wagner
Learning and Motivation, 2008
2002
How do humans discover causal relations when the effect is not immediately observable? Previous experiments have uniformly demonstrated detrimental effects of outcome delays on causal induction. These findings seem to conflict with everyday causal cognition, where humans can apparently identify long-term causal relations with relative ease.
Memory & Cognition, 2007
Thinking & Reasoning, 2006
Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, …, 2009
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.
Memory & Cognition, 2002
Memory & Cognition, 2004
Memory & Cognition, 2013
Acta Psychologica, 2008
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2007
Learning and Motivation, 2006
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2010
Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research
csjarchive.cogsci.rpi.edu
Learning and Motivation, 2006
Learning & Behavior, 2014
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2006
Memory & Cognition, 2005
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2005
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section B, 2004
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2006
Journal of experimental psychology. General, 2008
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2012
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2005
Acta Psychologica, 2018
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2006