Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
1984, Psychological Research
…
14 pages
1 file
Evidence for invariant relative timing is mostly based on comparisons between experimental conditions which differ in the average speed of the movements under study. The present experiment examines whether spontaneous variability of acceleration-time curves of aiming movements within a single experimental condition can also be described in terms of variable scaling parameters for both dimensions. This requires that the correlations between peak acceleration and peak deceleration, as well as those between time intervals defined by peak values and zero crossings, are high. In extensions of the elbow joint on a horizontal plane, the latter correlations turned out to be small or even negative. These results cannot be explained as being due to artifacts of several origins. To reconcile them with those based on comparisons between experimental conditions, a dual-level hypothesis of control in aiming movements is suggested. Only variability on one level can be described in terms of variable scaling parameters, and whether or not variability of acceleration-time curves can be described in this way depends on the relative contributions of both levels of control.
Nonlinear dynamics, psychology, and life sciences, 2009
When people perform repeated goal-directed movements, consecutive movement durations inevitably vary over trials, in poor as well as in skilled performances. The well-established paradigm of precision-aiming is taken as a methodological framework here. Evidence is provided that movement variability in closed tasks is not a random phenomenon, but rather shows a coherent temporal structure, referred to as 1/f scaling. The scaling relation appears more clearly as participants become trained in a highly constrained motor task. Also Recurrence Quantification Analysis (RQA) and Sample Entropy (SampEn) as analytic tools show that variation of movement times becomes less random and more patterned with motor learning. This suggests that motor learning can be regarded as an emergent, dynamical fusing of collaborating subsystems into a lower-dimensional organization. These results support the idea that 1/f scaling is ubiquitous throughout the cognitive system, and suggest that it plays a funda...
Acta Psychologica, 1997
The time required to program a movement response (reaction time) has been found to be directly related to the accuracy requirements of the response as well as to the number of movement segments comprising the response. However, since many of the experiments which have manipulated response complexity have concurrently manipulated the amplitude of the entire movement, it was not possible to determine which of these factors was responsible for the change in reaction time. The main purpose of the present experiment was to determine whether the time required to program a limb movement was affected by response complexity, by movement amplitude, by target size, or by some combination of these factors. Subjects made forearm extension and extension-flexion movements of varying amplitudes in the horizontal plane, to targets of varying sizes. The kinematic properties of these movements were also investigated to determine whether these movements were exclusively programmed prior to movement initiation or whether some form of on-line control occurred during the execution of the movement. Pre-motor reaction time was found to be dependent upon response amplitude and total movement duration more than it was on response complexity or target size. In addition, subjects adopted on-line control when the amplitude of the movement was increased and when the terminal target size was decreased.
Clinical Neurophysiology, 2011
The time course of joint angle variability during movement reveals important aspects of motor control. Similar to variance of hand position, joint angle variance of the arm peaks at around the mid of the movement and saturates afterwards. In the absence of strong accuracy demands, proprioceptive information provides most important feedback information for reaching control.
PLoS ONE, 2013
Our aim was to investigate how the organization of a whole body movement is altered when movement duration (MD) is varied. Subjects performed the same whole body pointing movement over long, normal and short MDs. The kinematic trajectories were then analyzed on a normalized time base. A principal components analysis (PCA) revealed that the degree of coordination between the elevation angles of the body did not change with MD. This lack of significant differences in the coordination was interesting given that small spatial and temporal differences were observed in the individual kinematic trajectories. They were revealed by studying the trajectories of the elevation angles, joint markers and center of mass. The elevation angle excursions displayed modifications primarily in their spatial characteristics. These alterations were more marked for the short rather than long duration movements. The temporal characteristics of the elevation angles as measured by the time to peak of angular velocity were not modified in the same fashion hence displaying a dissociation in the tuning of the spatial and temporal aspects of the elevation angles. Modifications in the temporal characteristics of the movement were also studied by examining the velocity profiles of the joint markers. Interestingly, unlike the disordered nature of this variable for the elevation angles, the time to peak velocity was neatly ordered as a function of MD for the joint markers -It arrived first for the short duration movements, followed by those of the normal and finally long duration movements. Despite the modifications observed in the kinematic trajectories, a PCA with the elevation angle excursions at different MDs revealed that two principal components were sufficient to account for nearly all the variance in the data. Our results suggest that although similar, the kinematic trajectories at different MDs are not achieved by a simple time scaling.
Experimental Brain Research, 2002
According to Fitts' law, there is speed-accuracy trade-off in a wide variety of discrete aiming movements. However, it is unknown whether the same law applies to cyclic aiming movements. In the present study, a comparison is made between discrete versus cyclic aiming movements. A group of 24 healthy participants made graphical pen movements in 12 different aiming tasks in which successive finger and wrist movements were emphasized, consecutively executed as discrete and cyclic movements and varying in three target widths. In the cyclic condition, aiming movements consisted of backand-forth movements that were performed in immediate succession for 20 s. In the discrete condition, back-andforth aiming movements were drawn as 20 single strokes, starting after a go signal and stopping after reaching the target area. The targets had various levels of spatial accuracy and the movements had different directions (from lower left to upper right; from lower right to upper left) elicit either predominantly wrist or finger movements. The amount of information processed per unit of time (bits per second; index of performance, IP), tangential velocity, the pen pressure, and the ratio of peak-over-mean velocity were studied to gain understanding about the differences in control between discrete and cyclic movements. It was found that the IP and movement velocity were almost twice as large in cyclic versus discrete movements. In contrast, the axial pen pressure and the ratios of peak-over-mean velocity were much lower in cyclic movements (1.24 N versus 0.94 N; 2.26 N versus 1.81 N). The results of our study indicate that the predicted constant IP does not hold for rapid cyclic aiming movements and that speed-accuracy tradeoff is different. It is concluded that cyclic movements exploit the energetic and physiological properties of the neuromotor system. Expected differences in brain activity related to discrete and cyclic aiming movements are discussed as well as several neurophysiological mechanisms, which predict more economic force recruitment and information processing in cyclic than in discrete movements.
Experimental Brain Research, 2006
Recent studies have shown that the initial impulse associated with goal-directed aiming movements typically brings the limb to a position short of the target. This is because target overshooting is associated with greater temporal and energy costs than target undershooting. Presumably these costs can be expected to vary not only with the muscular forces required to move the limb, but also the gravitational forces inherent in the aiming task. In this study we examined the degree to which primary movement endpoint distributions depend on the direction of the movement with respect to gravity. We hypothesized that the magnitude of an undershoot bias would be greatest for downward movements because target overshooting necessitates a time and energy consuming movement reversal against gravity. Participants completed rapid aiming movements toward targets located above and below, as well as proximal and distal to a central home position. Movements were made both with and without additional mass attached to the limb. Although movement time did not vary with experimental condition, primary movement endpoint distributions were consistent with our predictions. SpeciWcally, both greater undershooting and greater endpoint variability was associated with downward aiming movements. As well, a greater proportion of the overall movement time was spent in the corrective phase of the movement. These results are consistent with models of energy minimization that posit an inherent eYciency of control and hold that movements are organized to minimize movement time and energy expenditure and maximize mechanical advantages.
Journal of neurophysiology, 2000
A single force pulse was applied unexpectedly to the arms of five normal human subjects during nonvisually guided planar reaching movements of 10-cm amplitude. The pulse was applied by a powered manipulandum in a direction perpendicular to the motion of the hand, which gripped the manipulandum via a handle at the beginning, at the middle, or toward the end the movement. It was small and brief (10 N, 10 ms), so that it was barely perceptible. We found that the end points of the perturbed motions were systematically different from those of the unperturbed movements. This difference, dubbed "terminal error," averaged 14.4 +/- 9.8% (mean +/- SD) of the movement distance. The terminal error was not necessarily in the direction of the perturbation, although it was affected by it, and it did not decrease significantly with practice. For example, while perturbations involving elbow extension resulted in a statistically significant shift in mean end-point and target-acquisition fre...
Experimental Brain Research, 1999
Two experiments are reported that investigated the effects of target size and inertial load on the control of rapid aiming movements. Based on kinematic profiles, movements were partitioned into their preprogrammed initial impulse-and feedback-based error correction phases. Electromyographic (EMG) rise rates were examined to investigate whether participants used a speed-sensitive or speed-insensitive control strategy. The results from both experiments showed that initial impulse velocity and EMG rise rates varied as a function of target size, i.e., a speed-sensitive strategy. This was the case whether participants were allowed to make error corrections to their movements (experiment 1) or were instructed to produce initial impulses that hit the target (experiment 2). Both experiments also showed that initial impulse velocity and endpoint variability were inversely related to inertial load. The results from experiment 2 indicated that, while the manipulation of inertial load had no effect on EMG rise rates for movements to a large target, EMG slopes were modulated between inertial load conditions when the target was small.
PLoS ONE, 2011
The present study investigates how the CNS deals with the omnipresent force of gravity during arm motor planning. Previous studies have reported direction-dependent kinematic differences in the vertical plane; notably, acceleration duration was greater during a downward than an upward arm movement. Although the analysis of acceleration and deceleration phases has permitted to explore the integration of gravity force, further investigation is necessary to conclude whether feedforward or feedback control processes are at the origin of this incorporation. We considered that a more detailed analysis of the temporal features of vertical arm movements could provide additional information about gravity force integration into the motor planning. Eight subjects performed single joint vertical arm movements (45u rotation around the shoulder joint) in two opposite directions (upwards and downwards) and at three different speeds (slow, natural and fast). We calculated different parameters of hand acceleration profiles: movement duration (MD), duration to peak acceleration (D PA), duration from peak acceleration to peak velocity (D PA-PV), duration from peak velocity to peak deceleration (D PV-PD), duration from peak deceleration to the movement end (D PD-End), acceleration duration (AD), deceleration duration (DD), peak acceleration (PA), peak velocity (PV), and peak deceleration (PD). While movement durations and amplitudes were similar for upward and downward movements, the temporal structure of acceleration profiles differed between the two directions. More specifically, subjects performed upward movements faster than downward movements; these direction-dependent asymmetries appeared early in the movement (i.e., before PA) and lasted until the moment of PD. Additionally, PA and PV were greater for upward than downward movements. Movement speed also changed the temporal structure of acceleration profiles. The effect of speed and direction on the form of acceleration profiles is consistent with the premise that the CNS optimises motor commands with respect to both gravitational and inertial constraints.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.
Gait & Posture, 2006
Experimental Brain Research, 1999
Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology, 2011
Journal of Experimental …, 2009
Human Movement Science, 1995
Experimental Brain Research, 2006
Human Movement Science, 1999
Experimental Brain Research, 1994
Attention and Performance XIII, 2018
Experimental Brain Research, 2001
Human Movement Science, 2012
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1997
Psychological Review, 1988
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2000